This section outlines the future directions that research on lower “tar” and nicotine cigarettes should take. These are: (1) to perform additional laboratory studies under controlled experimental conditions, (2) to conduct additional research on compensatory smoking; and (3) to investigate both the biological and psychological factors involved in smoking. Research Priorities Controlled Studies To Determine the Role of Nicotine as a Primary Reinforcer in Cigarette Smoking Many important questions on the pharmacological importance of nicotine in maintaining cigarette smoking remain unanswered, despite a large number of studies on the topic (1, 2, 19, 25, 36, 44, 46, 49, 65, 69, 73). Nicotine is probably the primary source of the pharmacodynamic appeal of tobacco, but not enough is known about its exact role in smoking to determine whether it is the only source. (For reviews on nicotine and smoking, see 18, 21, 31, 57, 61.) Tobacco without nicotine appears not to be sufficiently reinforcing to support sustained use (18). There has never been an appropriately designed study with a large number of subjects randomly assigned to smoke flavor-balanced cigarettes of varying nicotine content over a substantial (months) time period. The behavioral aspects of cigarette smoking are of paramount importance in the evaluation of less hazardous cigarettes. Behavior is the interface between cigarette smoking, its pharmacological and physiological effects, and the generation of disease. Compensation for nominally reduced machine- measured “tar” and nicotine yields of cigarettes by increased depth and volume of inhalation as well as proportion of the burning cigarette consumed has been demonstrated. Such a study would be necessary to conclusively support this hypothesis of cigarette habituation. Instead, we can only look at the distribution of smoking by nicotine yield and the experimental literature. In 1979, the percentage of current regular smokers in the United States who smoked cigarettes low in nicotine content (less than 0.5 mg nicotine and less than 5 mg “tar”) was very small, about 4 percent. Research studies using tobacco cigarettes virtually free of nicotine show these to be rated as aversive by smokers (86, 64). At the same time, it has been difficult to demonstrate that smokers will use nicotine in a nontobacco medium. In one study, lettuce leaf cigarettes injected with nicotine were smoked for 1-week periods at intake levels only approximately 50 percent the rate of the subject’s own brand, and with protest of much reduced satisfaction (18). Considered a more direct route of administration, injections of nicotine became a satisfying replacement for cigarettes 7 after repeated trials, but this early study was not conducted in a “blind” fashion (38). More recent studies of intravenously administered nicotine have contained subjective reports of perceived pleasure (89), but also have included reports of an inability to suppress subsequent smoking to a major extent (39, 46, 49). Although the results were perceived as only mildly pleasurable, nicotine administered in oral tablet form ($5) or embedded in chewing gum (44, 64) has decreased various measures of smoking in individuals not trying to quit. The major problem with giving nicotine in other than inhaled form is that it lacks some of the biological as well as many of the behavioral similarities to smoking. The nicotine bolus, when inhaled, reaches the central nervous system in less than 8 seconds (58). More information is needed to understand the pharmacological, psychological, and situational cofactors that may contribute to the reinforcing effects of nicotine. By analyzing the mechanisms whereby nicotine reinforces smoking behavior, it may be possible to design more efficacious treatments for cigarette dependence or to devise techniques for maximizing the rewards of smoking while minimizing the risks to health. Animal Models of Nicotine Use Animal models have several advantages over human models in studying the effects of nicotine. In the animal laboratory, environmen- tal variables can be controlled to a much greater extent than they can in the human laboratory. History of exposure to the drug can be manipulated in a true experimental fashion. One of the greatest limitations of much epidemiological and behavioral research on human smoking behavior is that the subjects are self-selected. Consequently, the research is inherently correlational rather than experimental. Correlational research can describe associations between variables, but it is often confounded by unmeasured variables (30). Animal models have been used to study the dependence liability and toxicity of many drugs (17, 75). The techniques used in analyzing responses to other drugs should be developed further and applied to the study of nicotine—and perhaps other substances in tobacco. Methods of administration can have a large effect on the pharmaco- kinetics of nicotine. Oral, intravenous, and inhalation modes of administration should be employed, but since smokers receive nicotine from inhaled smoke, the inhalation route is particularly important. Unfortunately, animals do not inhale nebulized nicotine or cigarette smoke in ways that are comparable to human inhalation patterns (53). Until reliable inhalation methods for animals are perfected, intrave- nous administration will have to be used in much of this research. 178 The Self-Administration of Nicotine by Animals Since people take nicotine on their own, an ideal animal model would be one in which animals take nicotine on their own. Attempts to get animals to administer nicotine to themselves have not been uniformly successful (17, 21). Maintained self-administration has been found in the monkey and the rat in some studies (6, 22, 47, 50), but not in others (82). Recent work has shown that under some schedules of reinforce- ment, monkeys will self-administer injections of nicotine (12). In order to discover precisely what variables are critical to the reinforcing properties of nicotine, further studies are needed. In addition to studying the parameters of self-administration, toxicity should also be measured. For example, it is important to look at the variables of physical dependence, food and water intake, and morbidity, as well as necropsy findings. The Study of Tolerance and Physical Dependence Both tolerance and physical dependence can develop to nicotine or other ingredients in tobacco ($8, 48, 71, 78). Animal models have been used successfully in research on opioids and alcohol (70) and could prove effective in future research on nicotine and smoking. Appropriate animal models would facilitate the study of the pharmacokinetics of nicotine and would help in the evaluation of pharmacological treatments for dependence. Since tolerance and physical dependence can influence the reinforcing properties of drugs of abuse, animal studies should investigate the extent to which withdrawal phenomena may contribute to the reinforcing properties of cigarette smoke. Methods developed for evaluation of opioid drugs could be adopted for these purposes. Nicotine Research With Humans The scientific issues in human and animal research are similar, although not all studies conducted on animals are practically and ethically suitable for research on humans. A great amount of preliminary data already exists on the role of nicotine in human smoking behavior (see the reviews cited above), but the influence of tolerance and dependence on nicotine on the initiation, maintenance, and cessation of smoking behavior are still not resolved (27, 46, 59, 61, 68). Clearly, both biological and psychosocial factors influence human cigarette intake (41), and it is in the human model of cigarette smoking that the interplay of these factors can best be studied. There is no known analog in animal behavior for future orientation and cognitive factors, such as worrying about the risks of cancer or about weight gain upon giving up smoking. Progress to date in laboratory studies of smoking dependence has been slowed by the lack of standardized test materials, such as 179 cigarettes made to research specifications, and of standardized, easily accessible laboratory analyses, such as for plasma levels of nicotine. Compensatory Behavior in Smoking If, in the course of a standard assay for the “tar” and nicotine yields of a cigarette (54), a smoking-machine derives relatively small amounts of “tar” and nicotine, the cigarette can be called lower “tar” and nicotine. Unfortunately the smoking-machine model is limited in accurately reproducing human smoking behavior. The machines take a 2 second, 35 cc puff each minute until a predetermined butt length is reached. Smokers, however, are able to take larger, more frequent, and higher velocity puffs than the machines do. It appears that such compensatory adjustments often turn nominally lower “tar” and nicotine cigarettes into higher “tar” and nicotine cigarettes (1, 4, 9, 25, 36, 46, 60, 62). Even if the compensations made in smoking a single cigarette are small or nonexistent, smokers can increase their intake of “tar” and nicotine by smoking more cigarettes (66). Cigarettes of less than about 6 mg “tar” and 0.5 mg nicotine are also subject to the influences of compensatory smoking. Most of these cigarettes achieve their lower yields as a result of ventilation holes placed in the filters, which cause each puff of smoke to be diluted with air. These air-diluted puffs deliver relatively small amounts of “tar,” nicotine, and carbon monoxide to the smoking-machines (29). Some smokers have learned to block the ventilation holes with their lips or fingers—or sometimes with tape—and thereby, often unwittingly, defeat the purpose of the holes. If the ventilation holes are blocked, yields of nicotine, “tar,” and carbon monoxide can increase by about two, three, and four times, respectively (42). In 1979, ventilated-filter cigarettes accounted for about 25 percent of total cigarette sales (29). Many studies have used estimates of nicotine and smoke intake based on direct observations (44), measurements of smoking topogra- phy by means of special cigarette holders (24, 36), or analyses of residual nicotine in cigarette filters (1, 9, 55). Only a few studies have measured the levels of nicotine in plasma as a function of the nominal smoking-machine yields (1, 63), but research indicates that some smokers do compensate for reduced yields of nicotine. By smoking more to compensate for lower nicotine intake, lower “tar” and nicotine cigarette smokers can inadvertently increase their exposure to “tar” and carbon monoxide beyond what might be expected from a less intensively smoked higher “tar” and nicotine cigarette (57, 67). Because less hazardous cigarettes may require the delivery of moderate levels of nicotine while delivering lower levels of “tar” and carbon monoxide, Russell (5 7) has proposed that lower “tar” to nicotine ratios should be used to indicate less hazardous cigarettes. These ratios may direct smokers to potentially less hazardous ciga- rettes, but the way in which a cigarette is smoked can affect the ratio 180 examination of the advisability of encouraging people to switch to milder cigarettes should be undertaken. (See Russell (60) for a brief discussion of the possible role of self-selection biases in the epidemio- logical finding that filter-tipped cigarettes are less hazardous (8, 81). See Harris (23) for a summary discussion of behavioral and economic factors affecting the promotion of lower “tar” and nicotine cigarettes.) Controlled Switching Very few studies on controlled switching have employed measures of plasma nicotine (1, 28, 60). No large-scale studies have been conducted that make use of plasma nicotine, carbon monoxide, and physiological measures of smoke exposure. The relationship between smoker satisfaction and compensatory smoking appears to be complex. One forced switching study (74) has shown that, even though the compensation was incomplete and did not change for the few days of the study, satisfaction did improve during the course of the experiment. We do not know if satisfaction with lower “tar” and nicotine cigarettes increases with duration of their use, if it decreases with time if compensation occurs initially, or if nicotine yield alone determines cigarette acceptability. Additional Comments As noted earlier, progress in compensatory smoking research has been hindered by the lack of research cigarettes varying systematically in nicotine, “tar,” and carbon monoxide, and by the shortage of laboratory facilities in which to do needed analyses. One byproduct of the proposed research on switching to lower “tar” and nicotine cigarettes might be the development of practical diagnos- tic techniques. Smokers and physicians have not determined whether lower “tar” and nicotine cigarettes have produced “low-yield” smok- ing, but simple measures such as expired air carbon monoxide (11, 26) might help supply needed information concerning smoke exposure. Natural History of Smoking Along Both Biological and Psychosocial Dimensions Since almost nothing is known about the role of lower “tar” and nicotine cigarettes at crucial transition points in a smoker’s history, this issue cannot be considered in detail (7, 20, 40, 52, 56). One key unanswered question is whether lower “tar” and nicotine cigarettes tend to facilitate taking up the smoking habit. Presumably, initiation of smoking is easier for those who first try lower “tar” and nicotine cigarettes than for those who first try regular cigarettes. Thus, lower “tar” and nicotine cigarettes can reduce aversive physical responses to early smoking episodes that might otherwise deter taking up the habit (43, 56). 182 Teenagers generally prefer moderately high-yield cigarettes (77), but 2.5 percent of the boys and 12.3 percent of the girls who smoke use lower “tar” and nicotine brands (here defined as < 10 mg “tar”). Research has not addressed the question of what percentage of these smokers may have been helped either in their initiation to smoking or in their shift from casual to habitual smoking by the use of lower “tar” and nicotine cigarettes. The incidence of smoking among teenage girls has increased during the past 10 years (76, 77). Silverstein et al. (72) present data supporting the hypothesis that the increasing availability of lower “tar” and nicotine cigarettes has encouraged this increase in smoking. Analysis of a survey of high school students suggests that girls experience greater social pressure to smoke than do boys, and that they also face greater physiological pressure not to smoke because of their higher sensitivity to nicotine. Girls appear to resolve these pressures by becoming lighter smokers than boys and by switching to lower “tar” and nicotine cigarettes. Perhaps if lower “tar” and nicotine cigarettes were less available, some girls would choose not to smoke rather than to experience unpleasant nicotine reactions. Most research on the initiation of smoking and casual smoking has been psychosocial. No doubt there are practical, if not ethical, constraints on studying biological influences on smoking among teenagers. Whatever the reason, very little is known, for example, about the role of nicotine in early smoking experiences. No one knows how much exposure (days, months, years) to smoking is needed before withdrawal symptoms appear. More balance is needed in research on teenage smoking. Whenever possible, biological factors—both physio- logical and pharmacological—should be studied along with psychoso- cial factors (27, 41). There has been little research on the effects of lower “tar” and nicotine cigarettes on maintenance or cessation of smoking. There are studies on the effects of using decreasing amounts of “tar” and nicotine as a cessation or reduction aid (10), but these studies do not include biochemical or physiological measures of change in smoke exposure. It seems plausible that the alternative of a supposedly less- hazardous cigarette might make some smokers less likely to try to abstain completely. By the same token, the example of a satisfied, though perhaps fully compensating, smoker of lower “tar” and nicotine cigarettes might make a former smoker more likely to relapse. The former smoker might view the lower “tar” and nicotine cigarettes as both acceptable and safe (14, 15). Answers to these questions can have immediate implications for smoking treatment. Research in this area should include such crucial variables as gender (72). Both experimental and epidemiological data are needed in these studies. Perhaps large- scale smoking surveys can be expanded to include more questions that would help characterize the natural histories of smokers. 183 Recommendations Clinical Testing Facilities and Standardized Research Cigarettes There has been an active research effort in this country on the behavioral aspects of smoking. To further its productivity and to refine the scientific questions that this research can address, especially with regard to lower “tar” and nicotine cigarettes, the facilities and research cigarettes described here are needed. Clinical Testing Facilities These facilities should be able to provide biochemical and pharmaco- logical analyses of assays for plasma nicotine, cotinine, carboxyhemo- globin, and salivary thiocyanate. (Jarvik (34) reviews the use of these assays.) Each of these assays can be used to measure a smoker's exposure to some of the toxic and/or reinforcing ingredients in tobacco smoke. Plasma assays for nicotine (8) are available in a few laborato- ries; these assays can require special facilities to avoid problems of contamination. For example, a laboratory that is used part of the time by a worker who smokes may be unacceptable for the evaluation of plasma nicotine levels. Few behavioral researchers have access to or sufficient control over the needed laboratory facilities. Laboratories of this nature would be a great boon to behavioral research and would help to standardize assays in this area. Research Cigarettes A supply of clinically acceptable cigarettes that vary in nicotine, “tar,” and carbon monoxide yield should be made available to behavioral researchers. Although some standardized cigarettes have been available for years from the Tobacco and Health Research Institute of the University of Kentucky, these cigarettes have no filters, and their lack of palatability and acceptability almost complete- ly precludes their use in behavioral research. Cigarette technology has several ways of altering “tar,” nicotine, and carbon monoxide yields. Ideally, different strategies would be employed to produce cigarettes with identical machine-smoked yields. Consider two examples. A fast- burning, strong-tobacco cigarette might have the same yields as a slow-burning, mild-tobacco cigarette, but it is not clear how human smoking behavior might change as a function of these modes of yield reduction. A cigarette low in carbon monoxide could be made with either vented cigarette paper or a vented filter. The vented filter can be closed by smokers accidentally or intentionally, thereby increasing the actual yield to the smoker (42), but the effect of porous cigarette papers cannot readily be circumvented by the smoker. Variations in “tar” to nicotine ratios should be of special concern (57). It is important to determine the lowest ratios that still produce a satisfying cigarette. Obviously, identical “tar” and nicotine ratios can 184 occur in cigarettes that have very different standard nicotine yields. Research could show if there is an optimum combination of standard yield and ratio that leads to maximum satisfaction and minimal exposure to toxic products. Cigarettes that vary systematically in “tar” to nicotine ratios are needed for this research. Machine-Smoked Yields of Lower “Tar” and Nicotine Cigarettes The standard smoking-machine assay of “tar” and nicotine yields provides inadequate information to the tobacco consumer as well as to the researcher (16, 45, 74). The published yields do not indicate how many puffs were taken on a particular brand (45); assays at the Oak Ridge National Laboratory (37) reveal that from 6.9 to 11.5 puffs are taken on different brands of king-size filter cigarettes during standard assays. The current smoking-machine standards are meant to represent an average smoker, but it is probable that the standard puff volume (35 ec) is too small (5, 51) and that the puff interval (one puff per minute) is too long (4, 74). Since compensatory smoking occurs with lower “tar” and nicotine cigarettes, larger and more frequent puffs tend to be taken. Smokers sometimes interfere with ventilation holes on lower “tar” and nicotine cigarettes (45); smoking-machines do not. In addition to the standard assays, there should be maximum-yield assays of “tar,” nicotine, and carbon monoxide. These assays would be based on puffing parameters of volume, rate, and duration for the 95th—or even the 75th—percentile of heavy smokers smoking lower “tar” and nicotine ventilated cigarettes up to the tip overwrap. These parameters would be used in smoking-machines, with these same ventilated brands, to derive yields with ventilation holes in both blocked and unblocked conditions. This procedure would produce much higher yields than does the standard assay, and these values would better represent the possible maximum risks of the lower “tar” and nicotine cigarettes to smokers who engage in compensatory smoking. Without access to information about how much the standard yields can change with intensive smoking, there can be only a limited understand- ing of possible reductions in actual smoking exposure. Using research in the British-American Tobacco Company Laboratories in the United Kingdom, Green (16) has argued that intensive smoking can make middle “tar” cigarettes (11 to 16 mg) deliver as much as high “tar” cigarettes (31 to 35 mg). Green could not demonstrate that low “tar” cigarettes (0.4 to 9 mg) can be made to deliver high “tar” levels, but this study did not consider the effect of blocking the ventilation holes on these cigarettes. Toxicology of Nicotine A probable outcome of behavioral research will be that nicotine is the primary pharmacological reinforcer for cigarette smoking. If this 185 prediction is correct, a lower “tar” and nicotine cigarette that will be used by smokers and that will minimize the exposure to other toxic components of smoke may require substantial yields of nicotine (57, 62). Consideration of the toxicity of nicotine, then, may become crucial in determining whether the benefits of lower “tar” and nicotine cigarette smoking outweigh the costs. Summary 1. Nicotine appears to be the primary pharmacological reinforcer in tobacco, but other pharmacological and psychosocial factors may also contribute a reinforcing effect. 2. It appears that some smokers make compensatory adjustments in their smoking behavior with cigarettes of different yields that might increase the amounts of harmful substances entering the body. The frequency and amount of Spontaneous compensatory changes in smoking style with different cigarettes require further investigation. 3. Additional information is needed on the role of lower “tar” and nicotine cigarettes in the initiation, maintenance, and cessation of smoking. 4. Rigorous comparative behavioral studies involving animals are needed to provide comprehensive, experimentally valid results on behavioral aspects of smoking. 5. Laboratory techniques developed for study of opioids and alcohol should be adapted for studies of tolerance and dependence on nicotine. 6. Improved laboratory facilities are necessary for more tightly controlled behavioral research. A particular need exists for clinically acceptable cigarettes with standardized ingredients. 7. Smoking-machine measurements that more closely simulate the practices of human smokers must be developed. 186 References (1) (2) (8) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9) (10) (11) (12) (13) (14) (15) (16) ASHTON, H., STEPNEY, R., THOMPSON, J.W. Self-titration by cigarette smokers. British Medical Journal 2(6186): 357-360, August 11, 1979. ASHTON, H., WATSON, D.W. Puffing frequency and nicotine intake in cigarette smokers. British Medical Journal 3(5724): 679-681, September 19, 1970. AUERBACH, O., HAMMOND, E.C., GARFINKEL, L. Changes in bronchial epithelium in relation to cigarette smoking, 1955-1960 vs. 1970-1977. New England Journal of Medicine 300(8): 381-386, February 22, 1979. CREIGHTON, D.E., LEWIS, P.H. The effect of different cigarettes on human smoking patterns. In: Thornton, R.E. (Editor). Smoking Behavior, Physiologi- cal and Psychological Influences. New York, Churchill Livingstone, 1978, pp. 289-300. CREIGHTON, D.E., LEWIS, P.H. The effect of smoking pattern on smoke deliveries. In: Thornton, R.E. (Editor). Smoking Behavior, Physiological and Psychological Influences. New York, Churchill Livingstone, 1978, pp. 301-314. DENEAU, G.A., INOKI, R. Nicotine self-administration in monkeys. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 14% Article 1): 277-279, March 15, 1967. EVANS, R.1., HENDERSON, A., HILL, P., RAINES, B. Smoking in children and adolescents: Psychosocial determinants and prevention strategies. In: Krasnegor, N.A. (Editor). The Behavioral Aspects of Smoking. National Institute on Drug Abuse Research Monograph No. 26. U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, Public Health Service, Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration, National Institute on Drug Abuse, DHEW Publication No. (ADM) 79-882, August 1979, pp. 69-96. FEYERABEND, C., LEVITT, T., RUSSELL, M.A.H. A rapid gas-liquid chromatographic estimation of nicotine in biological fluids. Journal of Pharmacy and Pharmacology 27(6): 434-436, June 1975. FORBES, W.F., ROBINSON, J.C., HANLEY, J.A., COLBURN, HLN. Studies on the nicotine exposure of individual smokers. 1. Changes in mouth-level exposure to nicotine on switching to lower nicotine cigarettes. International Journal of the Addictions 11(6): 933-950, 1976. FOXX, R.M., BROWN, R.A. Nicotine fading and self-monitoring for cigarette abstinence or controlled smoking. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis 12(1): 111-125, Spring 1979. FREDERIKSEN, L.W., MARTIN, J.E. Carbon monoxide and smoking behavior. Addictive Behaviors 41): 21-30, 1979. GOLDBERG, S.R., SPEALMAN, R.D. Maintenance and Suppreasion of Re- sponding by Nicotine in Monkeys. Paper presented at the National Institute on Drug Abuse Technical Review on Cigarette Smoking as an Addiction, Rockville, Maryland, August 23-24, 1979. GOLDFARB, T., JARVIK, M.E., GLICK, S.D. Cigarette nicotine content as a determinant of human smoking behavior. Psychopharmacologia 17(1): 89-93, 1970. GORI, G.B. Low risk cigarettes: A prescription. Science 194(4271): 1248-1246, December 17, 1976. GORI, G.B., LYNCH, C.J. Toward less hazardous cigarettes. Current advances. Journal of the American Medical Association 240(12): 1255-1259, September 15, 1978. GREEN, S.J. Ranking cigarette brands on smoke deliveries. In: Thornton, R.E. (Editor). Smoking Behavior, Physiological and Psychological Influences. New York, Churchill Livingstone, 1978, pp. 381-388. 187 (17) (18) (19) (20) (21) (22) (28) (24) (25) (26) (27) (28) (29) (80) (81) 188 GRIFFITHS, R.R., BRADY, J.V., BRADFORD, L.D. Predicting the abuse liability of drugs with animal drug self-administration procedures: Psychomo- tor stimulants and hallucinogens. In: Thompson, T., Dews, P. (Editors). Advances in Behavioral Pharmacology. Volume 2. New York, Academic Press, 1979, pp. 163-208. GRITZ, E.R. Smoking behavior and tobacco abuse. In: Mello, N.K. (Editor). Advances in Substance Abuse. Volume 1. Greenwich, Connecticut, JAI Press, 1980, pp. 91~158. GRITZ, E.R., BAER-WEISS, V., JARVIK, M.E. Titration of nicotine intake with full-length and half-length cigarettes. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics 20(5): 552-556, November 1976. GRITZ, E.R., BRUNSWICK, A., BIERMAN, K.L. Behavioral aspects of smoking. In: U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare. The Health Consequences of Smoking for Women: A Report of the Surgeon General. U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, Public Health Service, Office of the Assistant Secretary for Health, Office on Smoking and Health, 1980, pp. 271-359, GRITZ, E.R., SIEGEL, R.K. Tobacco and smoking in animal and human behavior. In: Davidson, R.S. (Editor). Modification of Pathological Behavior. Experimental Analyses of Etiology and Behavior Therapy. New York, Gardner Press, 1979, pp. 419-476. HANSON, H.M., IVESTER, C.A., MORTON, B.R. Nicotine self-administration in rats. In: Krasnegor, N.A. (Editor). Cigarette Smoking as a Dependence Process. National Institute on Drug Abuse Research Monograph No. 23. U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, Public Health Service, Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration, National Institute on Drug Abuse, DHEW Publication No. (ADM) 79-800, 1979, pp. 70-90. HARRIS, J.E. Public policy issues in the promotion of less hazardous cigarettes. In: Gori, G.B., Bock, F.G. (Editors). Banbury Report 8—A Safe Cigarette? Cold Spring Harbor, New York, Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory, 1980, pp. 383-340. HENNINGFIELD, J.E., GRIFFITHS, R.R. A preparation for the experimental analysis of human cigarette smoking behavior. Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation 11(6): 588-544, December 1979. HENNINGFIELD, J.E., GRIFFITHS, R.R. Effects of ventilated cigarette holders on cigarette smoking by humans. Psychopharmacology 68(2): 115-119, 1980. HENNINGFIELD, J.E., STITZER, M.L., GRIFFITHS, R.R. Expired air carbon monoxide accumulation and elimination as a function of number of cigarettes smoked. Addictive Behaviors (3): 265-273, 1980. HERMAN, C.P., KOZLOWSKI, L.T. Indulgence, excess, and restraint: Perspec- tives on consummatory behavior in everyday life. Journal of Drug Issues 92): 185-196, Spring 1979. HILL, P., MARQUARDT, H. Plasma and urine changes after smoking different brands of cigarettes. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics 27(5): 652-658, May 1980. HOFFMANN, D., TSO, T.C., GORI, G.B. The less harmful cigarette. Preventive Medicine 2): 287-296, March 1980. HULLEY, S.B., ROSENMAN, R.H., BAWOL, R.D., BRAND, R.J. Epidemiology as a guide to clinical decisions. The association between triglyceride and coronary heart disease. New England Journal of Medicine 302(25): 1383-1389, June 19, 1980. JAFFE, J.H., JARVIK, M.E. Tobacco use and tobacco use disorder. In: Lipton, M.A., Di Mascio, A., Killam, K.P. (Editors). Psychopharmacology: A Generation of Progress. New York, Raven Press, 1978, pp. 1665-1676. ($2) (33) (34) (35) (36) (37) (38) (39) (40) (41) (42) (48) (44) (45) JAFFE, J.H., KANZLER, M., FRIEDMAN, L. Studies of switching to low tar and nicotine cigarettes. In: Gori, G.B., Bock, F.G. (Editors). Banbury Report 8—A Safe Cigarette? Cold Spring Harbor, New York, Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory, 1980, pp. 311-324. JARVIK, M.E. Tolerance to the effects of tobacco. In: Krasnegor, N.A. (Editor). Cigarette Smoking as a Dependence Process. National Institute on Drug Abuse Research Monograph No. 23. U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, Public Health Service, Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration, National Institute on Drug Abuse, DHEW Publication No. (ADM) 79-800, January 1979, pp. 150-157. JARVIK, MLE. Biological influences on cigarette smoking. In: Krasnegor, N.A. (Editor). The Behavioral Aspects of Smoking. National Institute on Drug Abuse Research Monograph No. 26. U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, Public Health Service, Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration, National Institute on Drug Abuse, DHEW Publication No. (ADM) 79-882, August 1979, pp. 7-45. JARVIK, M.E., GLICK, S.D., NAKAMURA, R.K. Inhibition of cigarette smoking by orally administered nicotine. Clinical Pharmacology and Thera- peutics 11(4): 574-576, July-August 1970. JARVIK, M.E., POPEK, P., SCHNEIDER, N.G., BAER-WEISS, V., GRITZ, E.R. Can cigarette size and nicotine content influence smoking and puffing rates? Psychopharmacology 58(3): 308-306, 1978. JENKINS, R.A., QUINCY, R.B., GUERIN, M.R. Selected Constituents in the Smokes of U.S. Commercial Cigarettes: “Tar,” Nicotine, Carbon Monoxide and Carbon Dioxide. Oak Ridge, Tennessee, U.S. Department of Energy, Oak Ridge National Laboratory, No. ORNL/TM-6870, May 1979, 42 pp. JOHNSTON, L. Tobacco smoking and nicotine. Lancet 2: 742, 1942. JONES, R.T., FARRELL, T.R., III, HERNING, R.I. Tobacco smoking and nicotine tolerance. In: Krasnegor, N.A. (Editor). Self-Administration of Abused Substances: Methods for Study. National Institute on Drug Abuse Research Monograph No. 20. U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, Public Health Service, Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration, National Institute on Drug Abuse, 1978, pp. 202-208. KOZLOWSKI, L.T. Psychosocial influences on cigarette smoking. In: Krasne- gor, N.A. (Editor). The Behavioral Aspects of Smoking. National Institute on Drug Abuse Research Monograph No. 26. U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, Public Health Service, Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration, National Institute on Drug Abuse, DHEW Publication No. (ADM) 79-882, August 1979, pp. 97-125. KOZLOWSKI, L.T. The Role of Nicotine in the Maintained Use of Cigarettes. Paper presented at the National Institute on Drug Abuse Technical Review on Cigarette Smoking as an Addiction, Rockville, Maryland, August 23-24, 1979, 13 pp. KOZLOWSKI, L.T., FRECKER, R.C., KHOUW, V., POPE, M.A. The misuse of “less hazardous” cigarettes and its detection: Hole-blocking of ventilated filters. American Journal of Public Health 70(11): 1202-1203, November 1980. KOZLOWSKI, L.T., HARFORD, M.R. On the significance of never using a drug: An example from cigarette smoking. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 85(4): 483-434, August 1976. KOZLOWSKI, L.T., JARVIK, M.E., GRITZ, E.R. Nicotine regulation and cigarette smoking. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics 17(1): 93-97, January 1975. KOZLOWSKI, L.T., RICKERT, W.S., ROBINSON, J.C., GRUNBERG, N.E. Have tar and nicotine yields of cigarettes changed? Science 2094464): 1550- 1551, September 26, 1980. 189 (46) (47) (48) (49) (50) (52) (52) (53) (54) (55) (56) (57) (58) (59) (60) (61) 190 KUMAR, R., COOKE, E.C., LADER, M.H., RUSSELL, M.A.H. Is nicotine important in tobacco smoking? Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics 21(5): 520-529, May 1977. LANG, W.J., LATIFF, A.A., MCQUEEN, A., SINGER, G. Self-administration of nicotine with and without a food delivery schedule. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior 41): 65-70, July 1977. LARSON, P.S., SILVETTE, H. Tobacco: Experimental and Clinical Studies. Supplement III. Baltimore, Williams and Wilkins, 1975, 798 pp. LUCCHESI, B.R., SCHUSTER, C.R., EMLEY, A.B. The role of nicotine as a determinant of cigarette smoking frequency in man with observations of certain cardiovascular effects associated with the tobacco alkaloid. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics 8(6): 789-796, November-December 1967, MCGILL, H.C., JR., ROGERS, W.R., WILBUR, R.L., JOHNSON, D.E. Cigarette smoking baboon model: Demonstration of feasibility. Proceedings of the Society for Experimental Biology and Medicine 157(4): 672-676, April 1978. MOODY, P.M., GRIFFITH, R.B., AVERITT, J.H. Smoking history and puff profiles of hospital patients. In: Proceedings of the University of Kentucky Tobacco and Health Research Institute, Tobacco and Health Workshop Conference, Conference Report 8, Lexington, Kentucky, January 11-18, 1972, pp. 31-48. PECHACEK, T.F. Modification of smoking behavior. In: Krasnegor, N.A. (Editor). The Behavioral Aspects of Smoking. National Institute on Drug Abuse Research Monograph No. 26. U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, Public Health Service, Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration, National Institute on Drug Abuse, DHEW Publication No. (ADM) 79-882, August 1979, pp. 127-188. PIEPER, W.A., COLE, J.M. Operant control of smoking in great apes. Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation 5(1): 4-6, 1973. PILLSBURY, H.C., BRIGHT, C.C., O’CONNOR, K.J., IRISH, F.W. Tar and nicotine in cigarette smoke. Journal of the Association of Official Analytical Chemists 52 (3): 458-462, May 1969. ROBINSON, J.C., YOUNG, J.C. Temporal patterns in smoking rate and mouth- level nicotine exposure. Addictive Behaviors, in press. RUSSELL, M.A.H. Cigarette smoking: Natural history of a dependence disorder. British Journal of Medical Psychology 44(1): 1-16, May 1971. RUSSELL, M.A.H. Low-tar medium-nicotine cigarettes: A new approach to safer smoking. British Medical Journal 1(6023): 1480-1433, June 12, 1976. RUSSELL, M.A.H. Tobacco smoking and nicotine dependence. In: Gibbins, RJ., Israel, Y., Kalant, H., Popham, R.E., Schmidt, W., Smart, R.G. (Editors). Research Advances in Alcohol and Drug Problems. Volume 3. New York, Wiley and Sons, 1976, pp. 1-47. RUSSELL, M.A.H. Tobaceo dependence: Is nicotine rewarding or aversive? In: Krasnegor, N.A. (Editor). Cigarette Smoking as a Dependence Process. National Institute on Drug Abuse Research Monograph No. 23. U.S. Depart- ment of Health, Education, and Welfare, Public Health Service, Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration, National Institute on Drug Abuse, DHEW Publication No. (ADM) 79-800, January 1979, pp. 100-122. RUSSELL, M.A.H. The case for medium-nicotine, low-tar, low-carbon monoxide cigarettes. In: Gori, G.B., Bock, F.G. (Editors). Banbury Report $—A Safe Cigarette? Cold Spring Harbor, New York, Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory, 1980, pp. 297-310. RUSSELL, M.A.H., FEYERABEND, C. Cigarette smoking: A dependence on high-nicotine boli. Drug Metabolism Reviews 8(1): 29-57, 1978, (62) (68) (64) (85) (66) (67) (68) (69) (70) (71) (72) (78) (74) (75) (76) RUSSELL, M.A.H., JARVIS, M., IYER, R., FEYERABEND, C. Relation of nicotine yield of cigaretts to blood nicotine concentrations in smokers. British Medical Journal 280(6219): 972-976, April 5, 1980. RUSSELL, M.A.H., SUTTON, S.R., FEYERABEND, C., COLE, P.V. Addiction Research Unit nicotine titration studies. In: Thornton, R.E. (Editor). Smoking Behavior, Physiological and Psychological Influences. New York, Churchill Livingstone, 1978, pp. 336-348. RUSSELL, M.A.H., SUTTON, S.R., FEYERABEND, C., COLE, P.V., SALOO- JEE, Y. Nicotine chewing gum as a substitute for smoking. British Medical Journal 1(6068): 1060-1063, April 23, 1977. RUSSELL, M.A.H., WILSON, C., PATEL, U.A., FEYERABEND, C., COLE, P.V. Plasma nicotine levels after smoking cigarettes with high, medium, and low nicotine yields. British Medical Journal 2(5968): 414-416, May 24, 1975. SCHACHTER, 5S. Nicotine regulation in heavy and light smokers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 106(1): 5-12, March 1977. SCHACHTER, S. Pharmacological and psychological determinants of smoking. Annals of Internal Medicine 881): 104-114, January 1978. SCHACHTER, 8. Regulation, withdrawal, and nicotine addiction. In: Krasne- gor, N.A. (Editor). Cigarette Smoking as a Dependence Process. National Institute on Drug Abuse Research Monograph No. 23. U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, Public Health Service, Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration, National Institute on Drug Abuse, DHEW Publication No. (ADM) 79-800, January 1979, pp. 123-183. SCHACHTER, §., SILVERSTEIN, B., KOZLOWSKI, L.T., PERLICK, D., HERMAN, C.P., LIEBLING, B. Studies of the interaction of psychological and pharmacological determinants of smoking. Journal of Experimental Psycholo- gy: General 106(1): 3-40, March 1977. SCHUSTER, C.R., JOHANSON, C.E. The use of animal models for the study of drug abuse. In: Gibbins, RJ., Israel, Y., Kalant, H., Popham, R.E., Schmidt, W., Smart, R.G. (Editors). Research Advances in Alcohol and Drug Problema. Volume 1. New York, Wiley, 1974, pp. 1-83. SHIFFMAN, S.M. The tobacco withdrawal syndrome. In: Krasnegor, N.A. (Editor). Cigarette Smoking as a Dependence Process. National Institute on Drug Abuse Research Monograph No. 23. U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, Public Health Service, Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration, National Institute on Drug Abuse, DHEW Publication No. (ADM) 79-800, January 1979, pp. 158-184. SILVERSTEIN, B., FELD, 8., KOZLOWSKI, L.T. The availability of low- nicotine cigarettes as a cause of cigarette smoking among teenage females. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, in press. STEPNEY, R. Consumption of cigarettes of reduced tar and nicotine delivery. British Journal of the Addictions 75: 81-88, 1980. SUTTON, 8.R., FEYERABEND, C., COLE, P.V., RUSSELL, M.A.H. Adjust- ment of smokers to dilution of tobacco smoke by ventilated cigarette holders. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics 2A(4): 395-405, October 1978. THOMPSON, T., UNNA, K.R. (Editors). Predicting Dependence Liability of Stimulant and Depressant Drugs. Baltimore, University Park Press, 1977. U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH, EDUCATION, AND WELFARE. Smoking and Health: A Report of the Surgeon General. U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, Public Health Service, Office of the Assistant Secretary for Health, Office on Smoking and Health, DHEW Publication No. (PHS) 79-50066, 1979, 1136 pp. 191 (77) (78) (79) (80) (81) (82) 192 U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH, EDUCATION, AND WELFARE. The Health Consequences of Smoking for Women: A Report of the Surgeon General. U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, Public Health Service, Office of the Assistant Secretary for Health, Office on Smoking and Health, 1980, 359 pp. U.S. NATIONAL INSTITUTE ON DRUG ABUSE. Technical Review on Cigarette Smoking as an Addiction. Final Report Summarizing Task Force Meeting Sponsored by the National Institute on Drug Abuse, August 23-24, 1979. U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, Public Health Service, Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health Administration, National Institute on Drug Abuse, 1979, 14 pp. WALD, N.J., IDLE, M., BOREHAM, J., BAILEY, A. Inhaling habits among smokers of different types of cigarettes. Theraz , in press. WALD, N., IDLE, M., SMITH, P.G., BAILEY, A. Carboxyhaemoglobin levels in smokers of filter and plain cigarettes. Lancet 1(8003): 110-112, January 15, 1977. WYNDER, E.L., MABUCHI, K., BEATTIE, E.J., JR. The epidemiology of lung eancer. Journal of the American Medical Association 213(18): 2221-2228, September 28, 1970. YANAGITA, T. Brief review on the use of self-administration techniques for predicting drug dependence potential. In: Thompson, T., Unna, K.R. (Editors). Predicting Dependence Liability of Stimulant and Depressant Drugs. Balti- more, University Park Press, 1977, pp. 231-242. Section 8. LOWER “TAR” AND NICOTINE CIGARETTES: PRODUCT CHOICE AND USE CONTENTS Introduction The Growth in the Use of Lower “Tar” and Nicotine Cigarettes An Increasing Public Awareness of the Health Hazards of Cigarette Smoking Public Attitudes The Cigarette Profile Cigarette Choice and Smoking Behavior Overview Relationship of “Tar” and Nicotine Yields to Smoking Behavior Consumption Patterns Cessation Summary Addendum: Comparison of “Tar” and Nicotine Yields of Cigarettes in 1978 and 1979 Yields of “Tar” and Nicotine Correlation of Varieties Reported in 1978 and 1979 “Tay” and Nicotine Yields of New Brands in 1979 Applications to the Discussion References LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1.—Market share of dollars expended in the U.S. on advertising and promotion of cigarettes yielding <15 mg “tar” compared with total domestic cigarette advertising and promotional expenditures for years 1970, 1975, 1976, 1977, and 1978 195 Figure 2.—Domestie market share of cigarettes yielding <15 mg “tar,” 1967-1978 Figure 3.—Sales-weighted averages of “tar” and nicotine per cigarette consumed in the US., 1954-1975 Figure 4.—Sales-weighted averages of “tar” and nicotine per cigarette consumed in the U.S., 1968-1978 Figure 5.—Ratio of “tar” to nicotine based on sales- weighted averages of cigarettes consumed in the US., 1968-1978 Figure 6.—Delivery of nicotine as a function of “tar” of commercial cigarettes, U.S. Figure 7.—Delivery of carbon monoxide as a function of “tar” of selected U.S. commercial cigarettes Figure 8.—Index of average “tar” and tobacco per cigarette, annually, 1967-1979 Figure 9.—Annual per capita consumption of total cigarettes and filter-tipped cigarettes in the U.S., for persons aged 18 and older, 1900-1979 Figure 10.—Annual per capita consumption of tobacco products in the U.S, (including overseas forces) for persons aged 18 and older, 1925-1979 Figure 11.—Annual per capita consumption of cigarettes in the U.S. (including overseas forces) for persons aged 18 and older, 1963-1978 LIST OF TABLES Table 1.—Estimated percentage distribution of current regular smokers by “tar” yield of primary brand of cigarette, by Sex, race, age, and education, adults, U.S., 1979 196 Table 2.—Mean daily dose of “tar” or nicotine for current regular smokers by race, sex, and age, U.S., 1979 Table 3.—Estimated percentage distribution of current regular smokers by “tar” and nicotine yield of primary cigarette used, U.S., 1978 and 1979 Table 4._Estimated percentage distribution of regular smokers by “tar” yield, adolescents aged 12-18, US., 1974 and 1979 Table 5.—Percent distribution of adolescent regular smokers by “tar” yield of primary brand, by sex and age, U.S., 1979 | Table 6.—Mean age at onset of regular smoking by “tar” or nicotine yield of primary brand, by age at interview, current regular smokers, U.S., 1979 Table 7.—Estimated percentage distribution of current regular smokers by number of cigarettes smoked daily by approximate quintiles of “tar” or nicotine yield, adults, U.S., 1979 Table 8.—Mean number of cigarettes smoked daily by “tar” or nicotine yield, by age groups, current regular smokers, adults, U.S., 1979 Table 9.—Estimated percent of current regular smokers who have tried seriously at least once to quit, by “tar” or nicotine level and age, U.S., 1979 Table 10.—Estimated percentage distribution of recent smokers by status of recent attempt to quit, by “tar” or nicotine yield of primary brand, July 1978 through December 1979 Table 11.—Estimated percentage distribution of current regular smokers by number of serious attempts to quit smoking, by “tar” or nicotine level, U.S., 1979 Table 12.—Mean duration of most recent attempt to quit, by “tar” or nicotine yield of current primary brand, current regular smokers, 1979 197 Table 13.—Mean yield of “tar” and nicotine of cigarettes, by type of modifier, all and filtertip varieties, U.S., 1978 and 1979 Table 14.—Mean yield of “tar” and nicotine of the varieties of cigarette marketed in both 1978 and 1979, by type of modifier, all and filtertip varieties, U.S. Table 15.—Comparison of “tar” and nicotine yield on the varieties of cigarette marketed in both 1978 and 1979, US. Table 16.—Mean yield of “tar” and nicotine of the new varieties of cigarette marketed in 1979, by type of modifier, U.S. Table 17.—Distribution of “tar” and nicotine yield of the new varieties of cigarette marketed in 1979, U.S. 198 Introduction This section discusses changes in cigarette smoking over recent years in the United States. Currently available evidence indicates that, while the prevalence of cigarette smoking is at its lowest point in several decades among both adults and adolescents, there are significant differences in the cigarettes being used by those persons who do smoke. The discussion does not attempt to describe comprehensively the patterns of cigarette smoking; such reviews have been published previously (25, 26). Rather, it focuses on the information that describes the cigarette products currently being used by smokers and the role that such modified products may play in the smoking habit. It includes examination of (1) the growth of the lower “tar” and nicotine cigarette, including social and marketplace activity in recent years, and (2) cigarette product choice and use by the smoking population. This consideration of changes in the cigarette is restricted to that of “tar” and nicotine yields of various cigarette brands, because of the availability of systematic measures of these constituents through the annual reports of the Federal Trade Commission (FTC). Note should be taken, however, that the extensive discussion of “tar” yields in this Report ought not to be construed as implying a primary or singular role of “tar” in causation of all the adverse health effects associated with cigarette smoking. Rather, “tar” yields are used because they are readily available and correlate closely to nicotine levels. No comparable measurements are available for carbon monoxide or other constituents of cigarette smoke, such as acrolein, hydrogen cyanide, or the nitrogen oxides, which are identified as probable contributors to smoking- related disease. Further, there are no systematic data available regarding the effects of commercially used cigarette additives on the yields of any of these constituents. Although the data cited here are derived from multiple sources, much of it represents the first analysis of a large, ongoing national survey. At the request of the Office on Smoking and Health, a smoking supplement was added to the continuing National Health Interview Survey (NHIS) by the National Center for Health Statistics. Begun in July 1978 and continued through 1979, the smoking supplement was designed to provide data on the prevalence of smoking, amount smoked, and attempts to quit smoking. Representing a random one- third subsample of the NHIS interviews of noninstitutionalized persons aged 17 or older, the 18-month data included approximately 36,000 individuals interviewed. Unless otherwise indicated, the data cited represent analysis of the approximately 24,000 interviews on smoking conducted during calendar year 1979. Lifetime smoking status (i.e, never, regular, occasional, and former smoker), age at onset, brand choice, amount smoked, and data on the attempt(s) to quit were collected for recent and current smokers. The “tar” and nicotine yields 199 for the 1978 and 1979 NHIS data sets are based on the FTC listing of cigarette varieties, sampled in 1977 and published in May 1978, and updated to include cigarettes identified by the FTC as marketed in July 1978. Three major conclusions can be elicited. First, Government and other agency activities in recent decades have led to widespread public recognition of the health hazards of smoking cigarettes, Second, the marked increase in the use of filter-tipped cigarettes in the late 1950s has been followed by a reduction in the “tar” and nicotine content of the cigarette products actually being selected and used by the smoking population. Third, the role of cigarettes of varying levels of “tar” and nicotine in the initiation, maintenance, or cessation of smoking js unknown. The data from the National Center for Health Statistics presented here neither prove nor disprove a role of lower “tar” and nicotine cigarettes in easing initiation, increasing daily consumption among regular smokers, or decreasing the probability of attempting to quit or of succeeding in the attempt. Much further work remains to be done to clarify and define the effects of lower “tar” and nicotine cigarettes on these behaviors, and thus their effect on total lifetime patterns of cigarette smoke exposure. The Growth in the Use of Lower “Tar” and Nicotine Cigarettes An Increasing Public Awareness of the Health Hazards of Cigarette Smoking The decades since the first medical reports of a link between lung cancer and smoking in the 1950s have seen multiple changes in the cigarette products being used by the smoking population (7, 14, 15, 16, 25). A number of factors may have encouraged these changes. The U.S. Public Health Service (PHS) has been active in assessing and attempting to reduce the excess burden of preventable illness related to cigarette smoking. Its first comprehensive review of the evidence linking cigarette smoking and adverse health effects by the Advisory Committee to the Surgeon General of the Public Health Service in 1964 was followed by regularly issued reports from 1966 through 1980, each of which continued and extended the PHS concern. In 1966, the PHS submitted to Congress (42) the Technical Report on “Tar” and Nicotine. On the basis of the clear demonstration of cigarette dose-dependent risks of several diseases, the PHS concluded: The preponderance of scientific evidence strongly suggests that the lower the “tar” and nicotine content of cigarette smoke, the less harmful would be the effect. We recommend . . . the progressive reduction of the “tar” and nicotine content of cigarette smoke. 200